1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 334 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

1. Evolution, Ecology and Behavior: The Nature of Evolution- Selection, Inheritance, and History


Poziom:

Temat: Edukacja

Prof: Biological evolution has two big ideas.
One of them has to do with how the process occurs,
and that's called microevolution.
It's evolution going on right now.
Evolution is going on in your body right now.
You've got about 10^(13th) bacteria in each gram of your
feces, and they have enough mutations in them to cover the
entire bacterial genome.
Every time you flush the toilet, you flush an entire new
set of information on bacterial genomes down the toilets.
It's going on all the time.
Now, the other major theme is macroevolution.
This process of microevolution has created a history,
and the history also constrains the process.
The process has been going on for 3.8 billion years.
It has created a history that had unique events in it,
and things happened in that history that now constrain
further microevolution going on today.
That's one of the tricky things about evolution.
It has many different scales.
My wife always gets frustrated with me.
She says, "Well when did that happen?"
I say, "Oh not too long ago, only about 20 million
years."
And, you know, that's what happens when you
become an evolutionary biologist, you zoom in and out
of deep time a lot.
And this process of microevolution is going to be
the first thing we examine.
It's the nuts and bolts.
It's what's really created the patterns.
But the patterns of macroevolution are also very
important because they record the history of life on the
planet and they constrain the current process.
So the evolution part of the course is set up basically with
two introductory lectures.
Then I'm going to spend six lectures talking about
microevolutionary principles.
So these are things that you can always return to if you are
puzzled about a problem.
Then there'll be five lectures on how organisms are designed
for reproductive success.
This includes cool stuff like sexual selection,
mate choice, that kind of stuff.
I usually manage to give the sexual selection lecture just
about on Valentine's Day.
Then we'll do macroevolutionary principles.
This has to do both with speciation,
how new species form, and with how biologists now
analyze the tree of life to try to understand and infer the
history of life on the planet.
Then we'll take a look at that history,
looking at key events--and this includes both fossils and the
diversity of organisms-- and some abstract organizing
principles about life.
So all of those are part of how we can analyze the history of
life on the planet.
And then, just before Spring Break, we will integrate micro
and macroevolution.
We'll do it in two different ways.
We'll do it with co-evolution, where micro and macro come
together, and we'll also do it with
evolutionary medicine, where both kinds of thinking
are necessary really to understand disease and the
design of the human body.
So where did this idea of evolution come from?
Well, there are always ideas.
You can go back to Aristotle and find elements of
evolutionary thought in Aristotle.
But really it's a nineteenth century idea,
and in order to see how it developed let's go back to about
1790 or 1800; so at the end of the Century of
the Enlightenment.
At that point, if you were to ask a
well-educated person living in a Western culture how old the
world is, they would say,
"Oh thousands of years."
And if you were to ask them, "Well, where did all these
species on the planet come from?"
they would say they were all created just the way they look
now and they've never changed.
And if you asked them, "Have there ever been any
species that went extinct?"
they would say, "No, everything that was
created is still alive and can be found somewhere on the
planet."
So when Alexander von Humboldt, who was certainly a creature of
The Enlightenment, sets out to explore South
America, he thinks that he might encounter some of those strange
fossils, that the French have been
turning up in the Paris Basin, on top of Tepuis in Venezuela.
So he really thought that there was a lost world.
Of course, Arthur Conan Doyle later wrote a novel about that.
But these guys actually thought, "Hey,
I go to Venezuela or I go to the Congo, I might meet a
brontosaurus."
That was what they thought at that time.
They thought that adaptations were produced by divine
intervention.
They did not think that there was a natural process that could
produce anything that was so exquisitely designed as your
eye.
We now know that your eye is in fact very badly designed,
but it looked pretty good to them.
Anybody here know why the eye is badly designed?
What's wrong with your eye?
Student: The blind spot.
Prof: It's got a blind spot and--?
Student: >
Prof: It's got--the nerves and the blood vessels are
in front of the retina.
The light has to go through the nerves and the blood vessels,
to get to the retina.
The octopus has a much better eye.
Okay, now by the time that Darwin published his book in
1859, people thought that the world is very,
very old; how old they weren't sure.
We now know about four-a-half billion,
but at that point, based on the rate of erosion of
mountains and on the saltiness of the ocean,
assuming that the ocean had been accumulating salt
continuously, and that it hadn't been getting
buried anywhere, which it does,
people thought hundreds of millions of years.
They weren't yet in the billions range,
but they thought hundreds of millions.
They knew that fossils probably represent extinct species.
That was Cuvier's contribution.
He did it for mammal fossils in the Paris Basin.
Geoffrey Saint-Hilaire had had a big debate with Cuvier about
homology, and that was in 1830.
By the way, it was one that many people throughout Europe
followed very closely-- this was a very,
very key intellectual topic at the time--
and it was about homology.
Basically it was about the idea that Geoffrey Saint-Hillaire had
had that if my hand has five fingers then--
and a bat's wing has five fingers and the fin of a
porpoise has five fingers-- that that indicates that we all
got those five fingers from a common ancestor,
and therefore we are related because we had a common
ancestor.
So you could see that in 1830.
That's before Darwin publishes his book.
Okay?
Then of course we have the idea that adaptations are produced by
natural selection; and we owe that to Darwin.
And I will run through the process he went through between
1838 and 1859 very briefly.
This is one of the most important ideas about the nature
of life, and therefore about the human
condition, that's ever been published,
andI strongly recommend that, if you have a chance,
read The Origin of Species.
Darwin actually was quite a good writer.
It's Victorian prose, so it's a little bit like
reading Dickens.
But it's good stuff, he has a nice rolling style.
How did he come to it?
Well Darwin was a med school dropout.
Went to Edinburgh, didn't like med school;
loved beetles and became passionate enough as a
naturalist to become known, as a 22-year-old young man,
as a guy who might be a good fellow to have on an expedition.
And the British Admiralty was sending Fitzroy around the world
to do nautical charts and Darwin got on the ship.
So at an age not very much greater, or perhaps even a bit
younger than some of you, Darwin sets off.
He's 22 years old.
He wants to know how species form.
He has set himself that goal.
So he's ambitious.
He's set a clear goal.
The goal is to solve one of the most pressing problems that
biology has at that time: where do species come from?
Now the stimulus that he has is in part from Charles Lyell,
the geologist, who had discovered deep time,
and that convinced Darwin that there would've been enough time.
He stops in Argentina.
In the banks of a river in Argentina he can see giant
fossil armadillos, and then right on top of that
same bank he can see the current armadillos walking around,
up on top of the bank.
There they are; the live ones are right above
the fossil ones.
They look the same but--I mean, they look similar--but they're
not the same.
So there's some connection there.
He gets on a horse in Chile and he rides up into the Andes and
he sees marine fossils lifted thousands of feet above sea
level; clearly some dynamic process is
going on that had lifted those marine fossils up.
He doesn't know about continental drift
yet--right?--but there the fossils are.
In the harbor at Valparaiso he sees the effects of an
earthquake that had happened just before they arrived.
It was a big one.
It was probably as large as the earthquake that recently caused
the big tsunami in Indonesia-- so it was probably an 8.5,8.6
earthquake-- and it had caused an uplift in
the harbor of maybe 50 feet.
So he began to see the world as dynamic.
Things hadn't always been the way they are.
Then he goes to the Galapagos, and please navigate the
Galapagos website and have a look at some of these
differences.
The thing that Darwin noticed is that the mockingbirds are
different on the different islands.
If you go to the Galapagos what you'll notice is that if you
land on Espanola, the mockingbirds really want
your water supply, and they will hop onto your
head or your knee to try to get at your water supply.
But, in fact, the mockingbirds all look a
little bit different on the different islands,
and that's what Darwin noticed.
He could also see that that the marine iguanas look a bit
different, and the land iguanas look different.
Interestingly, he didn't notice the
differences in the finches, until he got back to England
and gave his collection to the British Museum,
and the ornithologists at the British Museum came in and said,
"Hey Darwin, do you realize that the finches
on these islands are different?"
And that was when he began to really see how many differences
could accumulate, how rapidly,
when you take a migrant from Central America and put it on an
isolated archipelago.
So he goes back to London.
He's been onboard ship for about four years.
He has a problem with seasickness.
He never again sets foot on a ship.
He doesn't want to go near the water after being four years on
this ship.
He had a few issues with the captain too, Fitzroy,
but mainly it was that he had a very bad upset stomach onboard
the Beagle.
He reads the Reverend Malthus on population growth.
Malthus's book had come out in 1798.
Malthus said basically that populations grow exponentially
but agriculture grows linearly.
Therefore populations will always outstrip their resource
base.
This convinced Darwin that all organisms are in a competitive
struggle for resources, and that that must inevitably
be the case.
He saw very clearly how powerful reproduction is at
generating exponential population growth.
We will come back to that in the ecology portion of the
course.
And we now know that organisms are in competition really
essentially not just over food resources,
they are in competition over anything that will get their
genes into the next generation.
So that can be competition for mates.
It can be competition for nesting sites,
competition for food; lots of different things.
But at any rate this primed Darwin's thinking.
So he writes down the idea of natural selection.
It comes to him in 1838; it's in his notebooks in 1838.
Basically, I'll run through natural selection in a minute.
It's a deceptively simple idea because the mechanism looks so
simple, but the consequences are so wide ranging.
Darwin recognized what the consequences were.
And he didn't publish immediately.
He did other things.
He went off and he worked five or six years on barnacles.
He wrote down lots of ideas about things unrelated to
natural selection, and he wasn't really jogged out
of this until a letter arrived in 1858 from Alfred Russel
Wallace, a young British naturalist who
had, in a fit of malarial fever,
had the same idea, in Indonesia.
And Wallace knew that Darwin had been thinking about these
things, and he sent Darwin a letter.
And at that point Darwin, British gentleman as he was,
had to decide whether he would do the sort of gracious,
honorable thing and let Wallace have the idea,
or do the honest thing, which, his colleagues knew,
was that he had already had the idea.
And what they decide upon is that they will do a joint
publication.
So if you go to the Biological Journal of the Linnaean Society
for 1858, which is in the Yale Library,
you can look up the back to back papers by Alfred Russel
Wallace and Charles Darwin in which the idea of Natural
Selection is laid out.
And then Darwin rushes his book into print.
So he has been working on a book that was probably going to
be about 1200 pages long, and instead he publishes an
abstract of it, which he calls "The Origin
of Species", which is about 350 pages long.
And it sells out on the first day, sold all 6000 copies on the
first day, and has remained in print ever since.
That's The Beagle.
Darwin slept in a hammock in the captain's cabin,
at the back of the ship, which rocked horribly.
And that's essentially all I want to do about the development
of the idea of Evolution.
Basically what I did was I wanted to give you the feeling
that there was somebody like you who went out and knew what a
deep problem was, and happened to have the luck
to get into a special situation where they were stimulated,
and came up with an idea that changed the world.
No reason it can't happen again.
So now I'm going to give you a brief overview of microevolution
and macroevolution.
Here's Natural Selection; here's Darwin's idea.
If, in a population, there is variation in
reproductive success--what does that mean?
Would everybody in the room raise their hand if they're an
only child?
Look around.
There are about five or six.
How many of you come from families with two children?
Lots.
How many with three?
Quite a few.
How many with four?
Quite a few, but not as many as there with
only children.
Anybody with five?
Yes, a couple.
Anybody with six?
No.
If we were, by the way, in the nineteenth century,
at this point there would still be lots of hands going up.
What you've just seen is the amount of variation in
reproductive success represented by the families in this room.
Variation in reproductive success basically means that
different families have different numbers of offspring,
or different individuals have different numbers of offspring.
Then there has to be some variation in a trait.
How many of you are under 5'5?
Raise your hands.
How many between 5'5 and 6 feet?
How many over 6 feet?
Lots of variation in height in this room.
So we got lots of variation in reproductive success;
lots of variation in height.
There has to be a non-zero correlation between reproductive
success and the trait.
On this particular trait there's been some research.
Turns out that taller men have more children.
I don't know whether that's just an NBA effect or what that
is but it turns out to be true in many societies.
So there is a non-zero correlation between the
reproductive success and the trait.
Then there has to be heritability for the trait.
The heritability of height in humans is about 80%.
So all of the conditions for natural selection on height are
present in this room.
All you have to do is go out and have kids and it will
happen.
So if you're ever in doubt about whether evolution is
operating in a population, go back to these basic
conditions.
You can always decide whether it's likely to be operating or
not.
We can turn natural selection off by violating any of these
four points.
If there's no variation in reproductive success--
for example, if there is lifetime monogamy
and a one-child policy, there will be zero-variation in
reproductive success if everybody just has one child;
of course some people will still have zero,
but that's about as close as you can get.
If there's no variation in the trait--if the trait is like five
fingers; there are very few people with
six fingers; there are some, but very few.
If there's a non-zero correlation between reproductive
success and the trait; if there is a zero correlation
between reproductive success and the trait.
We'll go into all the conditions for that.
That results in neutral evolution.
Okay?
Then things just drift.
Well have a whole lecture on that.
Or if the trait is not heritable, if there's no genetic
component to it, then it won't evolve.
So Natural Selection-I wonder why it's doing that?
Sorry- Natural Selection does not necessarily happen.
It only happens under certain conditions.
Essentially in this picture, this is what I've just told you
about Natural Selection.
If there's variation in the trait, represented on the
X-axis, and there's variation in
reproductive success, based on the Y-axis,
and there is a correlation between the two,
represented by the fact that I can just about draw a straight
line between these points, Natural Selection will occur
and it will push the trait to the right.
If all of these conditions, except the correlation,
occur--so you have variation in the trait,
variation in reproductive success but no correlation--
then you get random drift.
And these two situations result in radically different things.
This situation produces adaptation, it produces all of
the fantastic biology that you're familiar with.
It's produced meiosis; it's produced your eye;
it's produced your brain.
It's extremely powerful.
This situation on the right, the random drift situation,
is what connects microevolution to phylogenetics,
and it's what allows us to use variation in DNA sequences to
infer history.
And I'll get to that.
That statement right now is opaque.
Don't expect that one to be transparent at this point.
But two or three lectures from now I will go into that in
detail and you will see that we need to have a process of drift
in order to generate a kind of large-scale regularity that
gives us timing and relationship in macroevolution.
So both are driven by variation in reproductive success.
The difference is in whether there's a correlation between
the variation of the gene or the trait and the variation in
reproductive success.
If we have strong selection, we can get pretty amazing
things.
I could illustrate adaptation a lot of different ways.
I could do it say with the leaf cutting ants that were the first
farmers; they domesticated a fungus 50
million years ago and have been cultivating it ever since.
That would be one way I could do it.
I could do it with the exquisite morphology of the deep
sea glass sponges and how efficient they are at filtering
stuff out of the water.
I could do it with the design of a shark's body.
Lots of stuff.
I'll do it with bats, in part because when I was a
Yale undergrad I worked on bats in this building.
We had a guy that did research on bats at that time.
Now a lot of bats are insectivores,
and they will hunt moths at night, in complete darkness.
They do it with sonar.
The bat only weighs about say 50 to 100 grams,
and it is making a sound that is as loud as a Metallica
concert when you're standing right next to the lead guitar's
speaker system.
Okay?
Or it's as loud, if you like,
as a Boeing 747 taking off from a runway.
It's this tiny little creature.
It's making an incredibly loud sound.
It's 130 decibels.
It does that because the intensity of sound,
the amplitude of sound, decreases with the square of
distance, and it needs to detect an echo
coming back from the moth.
The echo coming back from the moth--
which by the way it can pick up at a distance of about 20 feet--
is about a million times less loud,
and it's only coming in about one to two milliseconds later.
So imagine, there you are, you've gone "woo"--
except a lot louder than that--and milliseconds later you
hear "click", and you haven't deafened
yourself.
That's exquisite.
It has all kinds of physiology in its ear to hear the returning
echo, and it can actually discern
whether or not it's looking at a kind of a fuzzy moth or a smooth
beetle.
The moth has all kinds of adaptations to try to get away
from the bat.
It hears the bat.
The bat's cruising around, the moth hears the bat.
The moth goes into a desperate spiral, diving towards the
ground--okay--the bat starts to swoop in.
There is a mite that lives in the ear of moths.
I think you begin to understand the problem that this mite has.
If the moth gets caught, the mite will be eaten.
The mite's solution?
It only lives in one ear.
If you collect moths and you look for mites in their ears,
you will find that they are always only on one side.
So the moth always has a clear ear so it can hear the bat.
There's stuff like this all through biology.
There's another kind of a bat, called a Noctilio,
hunts fish.
A Noctilio basically detects ripples in the water surface,
and then it swoops down and it gaffs the fish with its hind
legs.
It can detect a wire 1/10^(th) of a millimeter in diameter,
sticking 1/10^(th) of a millimeter above the water
surface.
When I was taking care of bats, I'd never seen a Noctilio.
I thought, "God, this must be the greatest bat
in the world."
About four years ago, on the Amazon,
my wife and I went out in a canoe, at sunset,
on a lake, just off the Amazon River.
It was starting to get dark.
All day long the kingfishers had been fishing on that lake,
and during the day the lake had gotten covered with a lot of
food that the fish wanted, but they were afraid of the
kingfishers.
As it got darker the kingfishers couldn't hunt
anymore and the whole surface of the lake dimpled with the fish
coming up to eat the food.
So their timing was exquisite.
They knew exactly how dark it had to get before they were
safe.
The fish came up and started to eat the food.
At that point--it was just shortly after sunset--the bat
falcons were still stationed around the lake.
You could see, up on the trees,
falcons sitting up on the limbs and making flights off of the
limbs.
About 15 minutes after the fish started to eat,
it got dark enough so that the bat falcons couldn't hunt
anymore, and at that point Noctilio came
out, and the water was covered with
hundreds of bats that were catching the fish.
They were catching the fish within a meter of us.
Now there are a couple of things about that story that I
think, uh, I'd like to underline.
One is that that entire community is exquisitely
adapted.
Every element in it knows when everything is going on and what
the risks are, and what the costs and the
benefits are.
The other thing is that I had benefited from a liberal
education, and when that bat came out,
and was flying around a meter away from my canoe in the
Amazon, my life was so much richer
because I had been waiting to see it for 40 years.
I had heard about it in a course at Yale.
I knew where it fit in.
I knew what kinds of adaptations it had,
and boy was I happy to see it.
So adaptation can be impressive.
Drift is something that actually appeals to the geeks
among us.
I have a geeky side too, okay?
Drift isn't such a morphologically or artistically
beautiful thing.
It's a mathematically beautiful thing.
Drift happens whenever there is no correlation between
reproductive success and variation in a trait,
and it produces patterns like this.
So here we start off with 20 populations,
and we start them all with a gene frequency of 0.5,
and we let meiosis--which is like flipping a fair coin--
and we let variation and reproductive success take their
course, and we just run these
populations for 20 generations, and you can see that there's
just about an equally likely distribution of end-states out
here.
So we all start off at 0.5, and it gets noisy as we go
along.
So this is an image of the process of drift,
and if any of these populations happens to get up to 1,
or down to 0, in terms of gene frequency,
the process will stop, because those are absorbing
states.
If the frequency becomes 1, then everybody's got it and
there can't be any change, and if the frequency becomes 0,
then nobody's got it and there can't be any change.
So that's what's meant by absorbing state.
Now to a first approximation, whole organism traits are the
products of Natural Selection.
Maybe not in the immediate past, but usually at some point
in the history of life, a whole organism trait will
have been under Natural Selection.
So it will have been shaped and designed by this process.
And to a first approximation, a lot of DNA sequences have
been shaped by drift.
So we see design in the whole organism and we see noise in the
genome--to a rough cut; lots of exceptions.
There are DNA sequences that have clear selective value;
in fact, there's a whole literature on this now.
If any of you want to write an essay on signatures of selection
in the genome, you can find lots of stuff on
that now, on how to recognize that a
chunk of genome has recently been under selection.
There are whole organism traits that have no apparent selective
value; for example, the chin.
The chin actually is the result of evolution,
operating on development, to take a face,
which is like that of a gorilla or a chimpanzee,
which bulged out like this and essentially flattened it out;
so that we are vertically much flatter than a chimp or a
gorilla, and as a result of this being
pushed back, something that was there,
but kind of covered up, stuck out.
So that's where the chin came from.
That doesn't mean chins were selective.
Now it may be that after they originated, that there could've
been a little bit of sexual selection operating on them.
But certainly the developmental process that originally produced
them didn't have to be adaptive.
It could just be a byproduct of something that was going on,
basically from the mouth up.
So the themes of microevolution are selection and drift.
Natural selection is driven by variation in reproductive
success.
The strength of selection is measured by the correlation of
variation in a trait with reproductive success.
When there's no correlation, there's no systematic change,
and then things just drift, okay?
Now macroevolution; the big scale process,
the big picture.
Well here are sort of the basic statements about macroevolution.
If anybody asks you, "What does this fancy word
macroevolution mean?", tell them basically
this is it.
There's one tree of life.
Everything on the planet had a common origin.
Everything is related to everything else,
with the possible exception of the viruses, which are too small
for us to decide; their genomes are too small.
The branch points in the tree, speciation events--that's when
new species were formed.
This history is marked by striking major events.
There have been mass extinctions.
There have been meteorite impacts.
There have been major changes in the organization of the
information structure of life.
And the biological disciplines that you may encounter map onto
this timeline.
So actually different parts of biology study different parts of
this process.
The tree looks like this.
This is the large-scale tree.
So at this scale, what you see here are the three
kingdoms of life, which are the bacteria,
the archaea, and the eukaryotes,
up here; the root's at about 3.7 billion
years, not million years.
And at one point a purple bacterium got into the
eukaryotes and became a mitochondrion,
and at another point a cyanobacterium got into various
plant lineages, three times,
and became a chloroplast.
So that's the large scale.
And you're probably searching around on that to find out where
you, the most important thing in the
universe are, and you're way up here,
on a little twig.
Okay?
Now if we blow that up and just look at the multi-cellular
organisms, multi-cellularity looks like it
originated around 800 million to a billion years ago.
And these are the fungi, these are the things we call
the plants, multi-cellular plants,
and then off in this direction we have got a fairly complicated
series of branches that end up with us being up here.
Okay?
The things that are--this was done by Tom Pollard,
at MCDB, about five years ago, and at that point the things in
yellow had genomes that had been completely sequenced.
Now there are hundreds of completely sequenced genomes.
So for the first two billion years of life most of the action
is down in the basal radiation.
So going on with bacteria, archaea and eukaryote ancestor;
single-celled things.
At that scale--we're just way up at a small twig on the
tip--and symbiotic events brought mitochondria and
chloroplasts into eukaryotic cells.
Already this is telling you something interesting about
yourself.
You are a community of genomes.
You are not a unitary genome.
You've got that mitochondria in you.
The main themes are basically that the speciation events that
have occurred, particularly over the last
billion years or so, have created a tree of life
that describes the relationships of everything on the planet.
Systematic biology, phylogenetics,
tries to infer the history of life by studying those
relationships.
And there's a real deep issue here of how do we infer the
tree?
The tree--organisms don't come with a barcode on their
foreheads telling us who they are related to.
We have to try to figure out who they're related to,
and when we understand the relationships,
then we know the history, because the relationships
define the history.
So we work with hypotheses about history,
and we test these hypotheses against each other and try to
come up with the one that's most consistent with the data that
we've got.
And they give us a historical framework within which we can
then interpret what's happened.
There are major events that have happened.
Briefly these are they.
Life originates about 3.6 to 3.9 billion years ago.
And, by the way, it seems to have originated
fairly quickly.
Within probably about 100 million years--
see I'm being an evolutionary biologist again--
within just a hundred million years,
uh, after water could exist on the surface of the planet in
liquid form-- so following the meteorite
bombardment, when the surface of the planet
cools down enough for water to be liquid--
life probably originates pretty quickly.
And arguably, within the first hundred
generations, the first parasites were around.
So those things happened pretty quickly.
Then eukaryotes and meiosis, which is how a biologist refers
to organized sex, happened about 1.5 to 2.5
billion years ago; multi-cellularity,
which gives us developmental biology, about a billion years
ago.
All the major body plans for animals appear to have,
with the exception perhaps of the, uh,
jellyfish and a few of their relatives,
they all seem to have originated about 550 million
years ago.
There was a near loss of life on the planet in the Permian
mass extinction.
We will study that later in the course.
You're welcome to write an essay on mass extinctions if you
want to; you know, big death is kind of
exciting.
It seems to have occurred basically by a process of
poisoning of the oceans.
The flowers radiate about between 65 and 135 million years
ago.
Language is important because once language occurs,
then we have an independent kind of information transmission
from generation to generation; we get cultural transmission.
That's probably about 60-100,000 years old;
at least with syntax and complicated information storage.
Writing is only about 6000 years old.
And of course the important stuff is quite recent.
So this is a view of life that goes from bacteria to dinosaurs
to rock and roll; and that all can be studied
with evolutionary principles.
How do the biological disciplines map onto this?
Well microbiology and biochemistry try to study things
that are common to all life.
That means that the same chemical reactions that go on in
bacteria go on in the human liver,
and that's about one-and-a-half to four billion years old.
Okay?
Genetics and cell biology study stuff that follows the
evolutionary invention of meiosis;
to a large degree.
There is bacterial genetics, but eukaryotic genetics is
something which is studying things that are about 1.5
billion years old.
Developmental biology and general physiology,
those are multi-cellular disciplines;
they depend upon the existence of a multi-cellular organism.
That thing didn't come along until about a billion years ago.
Neurobiology, you need a complex--you need
cephalization--you need to have a complex nervous system.
That studies phenomena that are probably about 500 to 600
million years old.
Same for behavior.
There are several anthropologists in the class.
You guys are studying things that probably originated along
our branch of the tree, within the last 15 to 20
million years.
So there is a temporal assembly of biology, as a discipline,
as well as there is of life, on the planet.
So the key concepts from this lecture are that there are two
kinds of explanation in biology.
One is the proximate or mechanical question,
which is answered by studying how molecules and larger
structures work.
Those are basically physical and chemical explanations.
And then there are the evolutionary questions,
which is why does the thing exist;
why did it get designed this way?
And that could be answered either through selection or
through history; or the best way to do it is to
use both and combine those explanations.
The thing that distinguishes biology from physics and
chemistry is Natural Selection.
This is not a principle that you can find in a physics
textbook or in a chemistry textbook.
This is something that is a general principle that actually
applies to lots of things besides biology,
but it's not contained within physics and chemistry.
And there is a pattern in biology that unites biology with
geology and astronomy, and that's history.
So there is an important element of historical thought in
evolutionary biology, as well as the more abstract
action of natural selection on designing organisms for
reproductive success and shaping changes and gene frequencies.
Now I want to end the lecture by telling you something
astonishing.
I won't always be able to tell you something astonishing in
every lecture.
But one of the great privileges of teaching Introductory
Biology, or being in an Intro Bio class,
is that there are certain big things that never get discussed
again.
Okay?
This is one of them.
We are continuous with non-life.
Here's how I'm going to convince you of that.
Think of your mother.
Now think of her mother.
Now think of your mother's mother's mother.
Now I want you to go through a process like you've done in math
where you do an inductive proof; you just go back.
Just let that process go.
Okay?
Back you go in time.
Speed it up now.
Okay?
We're back at ten million.
Now we're at a hundred million.
Now we're at a billion years.
Now we're at 3.9 billion years.
Every step of the way there has been a parent.
3.9 billion years ago something extremely interesting happens.
You pass through the origin of life, and there's no parent
anymore.
At that point you are connected to abiotic matter.
Now this means that not only does the tree of life connect
you to all the living things on the planet,
but the origin of life connects you to the entire universe.
That's a deep thought.
Every element in your body, which is heavier than iron,
and you need a number of them, was synthesized in a nova,
uh, supernova.
The planet that you're sitting on is a secondary recycling of
supernova material, and your bodies are constructed
of that stuff and they use it in some of their most important
processes.
So the vision that evolutionary biology gives you is not only
the practical one of how to think about and analyze how and
why questions in biology, it's also a more general
statement about the human condition,
and I hope it's one that you'll have time to reflect on.
Next time we'll do basic genetics.
Mobile Analytics